Noesis
The Journal of the Noetic Society
Number 57
January 1991
EDITORIAL
Rick Rosner/Richard Sterman
5139 Balboa Blvd. #303
Encino, CA 91316-3430
(818) 986-9177
I received only one early issue of this journal before changing my identity.
As Richard Sterman, I received the last six issues of Noesis, which I
found pretty unreadable. When I agreed to edit Noesis for a few months,
Chris Cole supplied me with all the back issues.
I was surprised and intimidated to find much of the material in the older
issues readable and challenging. Chris Cole seems to have the most coherent and
optimistic vision of what this organization could accomplish; he has known
several Nobel Prize-winning thinkers and apparently hopes that we have similar
problem-solving capability. Cole has also supplied some of the logic puzzles,
especially Newcomb's Paradox, that have prompted the steadiest stream of
submissions, both readable and unreadable, from members.
I'm not as optimistic as Chris. He expects there to be few crackpots in this
organization. By any reasonable definition, I am a crank and perhaps not the
only one among members. Rather than anticipating consistently brilliant
tightly-reasoned articles, I'm also looking for loose, quirky, entertaining
material. Hoping to increase submissions from subscribers and to make Noesis
more interesting and less painful to read, I offer these suggestions:
1.
Length. Make it inversely proportional to the difficulty of your material.
Replace long words, if possible, with short, simple words. If you can, avoid
neologisms.
2. Rigorous and intricate arguments. If possible, separate the deep stuff
from the rest of the text so that the insufficiently-committed reader may skate
around it and still get the gist of your article. Put the easy superficial
stuff where casual readers can get to it. Assume that, in this journal, you
have an interested but impatient audience.
3. Religious subjects and unfalsifiable metaphysics (stuff that rests upon
faithful, hopeful assumptions). As popular as brussels sprouts. If you must,
give us small portions, keeping them light and irreverent. Pretend you're in a
crowded bus or a noisy bar and you're trying to get your point across without
everyone thinking you're a longwinded loon.
4. Material in poor taste or on questionable subjects. More, please,
especially if it will provoke responses from other readers.
5. Personal anecdotes. As many of these as you can supply, especially if
having a high IQ hasn't made your life any less absurd or pathetic (i.e. my
horrifying autobiography).
6a. Bizarre obsessions, speculations, calculations. Share 'em with us.
6b. Rules of thumb, observations, half-baked theories, pet peeves, ranting and
raving. Everyone, no matter how dopy, has a point of view, and few people
censor themselves. Why should having a high IQ stop you from going off
half-cocked? Let us read your screaming diatribe about every little thing.
Scribble it down and whip it off to Noesis. Can't we get into some
heated arguments about the Persian Gulf and boneheaded drivers, just like
normal people? [Now that I mention it--car phones--people who use them should
have them turn into steaming turds as they hold them to their ears. And why do
people drive like maniacs through traffic, zipping from lane to lane with
inches between bumpers, only to slow to a leisurely pace when they reach an
open stretch of road? And how can Victoria's Secret make a profit when they
deliver a new lingerie catalog every four days? And why, since 1987, do people
say "absolutely" or "definitely" when they mean yes?
("Is the Xerox machine plugged in?" "Absolutely!"
"Do you have dog feces on your shoe?" "Definitely!") Is
it a late-80's yuppie control-freak "I'm in charge" thing? Or does
it come from having seen too many old Federal Express "Absolutely,
Positively Overnight" commercials?]
Thanks for your attention. Don't hesitate to send (readable) stuff.
A reminder: TO CONTINUE TO RECEIVE NOESIS, YOU MUST SEND $10 FOR 6 ISSUES TO
RICK ROSNER, 5139 BALBOA BLVD #303, ENCINO CA 91316-3430.
WHEN
GOOD IQ'S HAPPEN TO BAD PEOPLE
(my appalling history with IQ tests)
Rick Rosner/Richard Sterman
1. GRADE SCHOOL
I was a nerdy, academically precocious little kid. Other neighborhood moms
criticised my mom for keeping me inside, working on learning drills, instead of
outside, playing with other kids. In reality, my mom was worried by my
precociousness and did little to encourage it. I stayed inside voluntarily to
avoid being tormented.
One activity with which my mom helped me, since she didn't fear it would teach
me anything, was drawing. We drew choo-choo's and traffic lights. My mom
would have been upset to know that she was helping me to score high on my first
IQ test.
On the last page of this test, given to everyone in my kindergarten class, was
the "Draw-A-Man Test." We got IQ points based on whether our men had
eyelashes, beltloops, and the right number of fingers. My man had all the
options. He was standing next to a traffic light. Several weeks later, at a
parent-teacher conference, Mrs. Shipper informed my mom and stepdad that I was
a genius, which they weren't thrilled to hear. It didn't bode well for an
already ill-adjusted child.
As a first grader, in addition to the aptitude tests administered to each
student, I was given a battery of tests to see if I should be skipped a grade.
I scored well above my grade level but wasn't accelerated, since it was
observed that I was "immature in behavior and a loner on playground."
My elementary school is located across the street from a state university with
a huge psychology department. Several times a year, youngsters who performed
well on tests were taken from class and used by psych students who had devised
their own aptitude tests. I remember sitting on a cafeteria bench next to
Brigitte Curtis while we were waiting to be tested. She is now a doctor. She
turned me down very nicely when I asked her to the dance where, as it turned
out, she was named Homecoming Queen.
When I was in second grade, my parents went to New York for ten days on a
buying trip for my stepafather's store and left me with Mrs. Clement, who was
old and frightening. I went nuts. When my parents returned, they found me
spinning clockwise (so that I wouldn't accidentally travel backwards in time)
and chanting to God. I was taken to a child psychiatrist and given more IQ
tests, including parts of a Stanford-Binet. I still have trouble turning left.
One of my happiest memories of testing is of fourth grade, when my class was
about to start an Iowa Test, which had to be timed to the second. Miss Garrett
was standing in front of us counting down the last ten seconds before saying
"Begin," when Becky Reynolds, the skinniest girl in fourth grade,
farted tremendously and turned crimson. Miss Garrett had to start a new countdown.
2. JUNIOR HIGH
While other boys were developing athletic and social skills, I stayed in my
back yard, working on a tan and taking dozens of tests from books entitled
Know Your Own IQ and Check Your Own IQ. I was a sucker for IQ tests.
I was the third biggest spaz in my junior high class but saved myself from
total geekdom by letting jocks copy off me (and by occasional bursts of
violence such as bouncing a padlock off of a mean upperclassman's head). Plus
I didn't eat boogers or have turds tumble out of my gym shorts as did other
class geeks.
Once, on a standardized test in eighth grade (the SRA Achievement--Green), I
surprised myself when, in trouble on one section, I copied off of John Lovell,
the most handsome guy in our class. I received a 99th percentile as always and
began to suspect that not all jocks were dopes.
In ninth grade, I was saved from another year of humiliation in P.E. when I got
a doctor to exempt me from gym because of huge varicosities in my lower legs.
However, hopeful of eventually losing my virginity or at least kissing
somebody, I started doing push-ups and pull-ups on my own. Having a high
hormone level caused in part by perpetual masturbation, I got strong fast.
My junior high offered an honors math class in which teams of two would work on
special presentations. My friend Lon and I were assigned statistics. The
stats we would analyze were the IQ's of the honors math class versus the IQ's
of the entire ninth grade. I was allowed to see the IQ's of everyone in my grade.
This did little for my mental health.
At 151, I had the highest IQ. However, my friend Lon had an IQ of only 118,
the lowest by far in the honors class. My teacher and I decided to tell Lon
that he had an IQ of 131.
As years passed, I worried about Lon. He continued to behave as if he had a
high IQ, doing well socially and academically, balancing more than a full load
as an undergrad in electrical engineering with getting laid a lot. When was
his IQ bubble going to burst, forcing him to gravitate to his true, lower
potential? I confessed to Lon, telling him his true score and asking him how
he could be doing so well. He in turn confessed that he had never understood
how he'd received a high score on that test, since, when the teacher said "Stop,"
he'd been desperately erasing answers, trying to repair his disordered computer
scan sheet after accidentally omitting an answer.
3. HIGH SCHOOL
I had three operations during my first three semesters of high school. I had
read Awakenings by Oliver Sacks and often stared at the TV while my mom watched
Marcus Welby, M.D., and I trusted my surgeon. I lifted weights to rehabilitate
myself, and after realizing that the surgeon had done a lousy job on two of the
operations, mangling my leg and leaving me with less sphincter control than I
would have preferred, I worked out with newfound anger and dedication. I
started wearing tight shirts and went out for varsity sports (with dismal
results).
I had the highest PSAT's in my class. I was on my way to Harvard, if only
because not that many people from my part of the country applied to Harvard.
But then I realized: everyone at Harvard had the highest PSAT's in their
class. Even with my weight-trained body, I was still a nerd at my small-town
high school--I'd be worse off, socially and intellectually, as part of
Harvard's cosmopolitan student body.
Through junior high and high school, I thought someone would love me for my IQ,
even though I was a nerd. But I got permission to examine my school records and
discovered that my IQ wasn't even that lovable. Most of my scores were in the
140's--not high enough to justify the special destiny I imagined. I decided to
try living as a regular dumb person.
4. HIGH SCHOOL
I broke into my high school, stole blank records, and created a school dossier
that said I was a year behind with a B average. After graduation, I left my mom
and stepdad and their conservative household and moved to another state to live
with my dad's out-of-control family and relive my senior year as a big, stupid
guy.
My new family was a hotbed of substance abuse and sexual intrigue, from which I
was largely excluded. Rules were more strict at my new school, the reigning
cliques were smaller and tighter, and I was thought of as a minor, sleazy thug
in disco clothes. I reacted by eating raw meat in chemistry and singing the
Masturbation Song in choir. I didn't have much fun, but being stupid turned
out to be familiar and comfortable.
5. COLLEGE
After less than three months I dropped out of my new high school and returned
to my original family. I continued to lift weights and eventually enrolled at
my hometown university where I hung out with other bewildered ex-jocks. I lost
my virginity to a woman who, I later found out, specialized in deflowering
nerds. She blew me off for a guy who robbed 7-11's. I mapped an escape route
from the 7-11 closest to my house but decided she wasn't worth it.
Though a born-again dumb person, I was still a sucker for IQ tests, and Kevin
Langdon's Adult Intelligence Test was a welcome excuse to ignore coursework.
It's a multiple choice test, and, since I couldn't decide between two answers
on one verbal problem, I submitted answer sheets under two different names.
Apparently one answer sheet was lost, but the report I received gave me a score
of 170, not far from the test's ceiling.
Heartened by the score, I began studying about intelligence tests. I
discovered that the relatively low scores I received in school were close to
their tests' ceilings--that few group-administered aptitude tests have ceilings
above 150. I felt redeemed, except that much of what I read indicated that the
relationship between intelligence and IQ is nebulous and that the history of
standardized testing is riddled with abuse.
I remained a sucker for standardized tests. I took the SAT's for other people
(with styrofoam blocks in my cowboy boots to pass for a 6'2" guy whose ID
I was using) and once on two hits of LSD as an exercise for a writing class.
6. FINDING SQUALOR TO MY LIKING
Even with my newly-inflated IQ, being stupid remained an attractive option. I
developed a stand-up comedy character whose self-esteem is shattered when he
discovers that he has an IQ of 76. (One of my classmates at the university had
had a junior high IQ of 67. She seemed normal.) My character, in college
because his dad pulled a few strings, decides that his brain is worthless and
concludes that his only value is as a big hunk of meat. So he becomes a bar
bouncer and a stripper. So did I.
I learned how to catch fake ID's, how to put people in a sleeper hold, and how
to do a floor show. Naked, I struggled to maintain my genitals in a proper
state of "fluffiness," neither too hard nor too soft. A pair of
ladies offered me 50 cents to see if I could get an on-stage erection (without
being caught by my managers, who were wary of vice cops). I held out for paper
money. I had been doing a lot of thinking about physics, and sometimes the
humiliation of nude dancing in a trashy dive forced me into a transcendent
state of focused, productive thought.
In one of the bars I was bouncing, I met a tall, strong, angry and underage
woman and we started dating. I broke off another incipient relationship by
giving myself a fake herpes sore with a wood burning set. I retired from
stripping and turned my G-strings over to another dancer. My girlfriend and I
had a heated relationship that grew more volatile after she, too, became a
bouncer. She was far better than me at being able to wrestle large, drunken
customers to the ground.
Among the things that angered my girlfriend was any evidence that I could
perform mental tasks which she could not. She considered herself intelligent
and hated that I might be considered moreso. People who remembered me as
something of a child prodigy really pissed her off. To avoid her wrath, I grew
skilled at censoring my thoughts and conversation.
She also hated that people had paid to see me naked. (They hadn't paid much.)
Eventually she dropped me and moved in with a guy who wouldn't trigger her
jealousy, another bouncer whose two years of steroid addiction had made him
stupid and bloated.
I missed the relationship, which had offered me the exciting possibility of
being trounced at any moment. I recovered from rejection by drowning myself in
work and obnoxious behavior. Every time I became depressed, I got another job,
until I had seven or eight simultaneously, bouncing three bars, stripping in
two, being a rollerskating waiter, a nude art model, and a library volunteer.
I sent full frontal shots to Playgirl, joined four gyms, and spent five weeks
taking Ron Hoeflin's Mega Test. I was probably the only person to work on the
test naked and contorted in front of art students.
My little brother attended my old high school. He didn't share my lack of
coordination (or my last name, which saved him from much embarrassment) and had
gone out for cross country to get in shape for JV basketball. Picking him up
one day after practice, I noticed five big boxes of papers out by the
dumpsters. They were full of school records. I returned later and crammed
them in the back of the Pinto.
For
some reason, Boulder High had held on to the files of five years of problem
students from the early seventies and thrown them out over ten years later. I
examined the mildewed and reeking records of hundreds of delinquent students,
saving the most classic examples of administrators' pitiless pigeonholing of
intractible teens. Once a kid was labeled, he was trapped in manilla, stapled
through the heart by grades and test scores, doomed. I ended up with a thick
composite file on the ultimate problem student.
Ron Hoeflin contacted me at the Chi Omega sorority house, where I was summer
caretaker, informing me that I'd tied for second among Omni's Mega Testtakers.
Already manic, I went wild trying to generate publicity. I wrote various
magazines, newspapers, and TV shows, claiming to be America's most deranged
genius. (I wrote The Weekly World News, telling them that abduction by aliens
raised my IQ. Muscle and Fitness readers learned that weight training had
increased my mental age.) At P.T.'s Showclub in Denver, I was billed as America's Smartest Stripper. My tips didn't increase, but a 240-pound woman was impressed
enough to sweep me off the stage and drive me back to her hotel room in a
rental car filled with junk food wrappers.
My mom showed me an article on Marilyn Savant from a midwestern newspaper in
which she was presented as fiercely trying to be seen as an ordinary person.
The article said she liked to date. I sent her some of my clippings, including
the feature that showed me taking off my clothes by setting them on fire. I
asked her if I could join the Mega Society and if she'd like to go out. She wrote
back, turning me down for membership. (My IQ fluctuated over and under the
membership cutoff as Ron Hoeflin renormed the test.) She didn't even mention
dating.
Playboy magazine ran a pictorial called "The Women of Mensa."
Though scornful of the organization, I rushed to join, hoping that Playgirl
could be persuaded to publish "The Men of Mensa." Before anything
could happen, the magazine was bought out.
I believe that Ron Hoeflin and Omni's Scot Morris were less than pleased at my
lack of respectability.* (I'd asked Scot Morris, since Omni is a sister
publication of Penthouse, if he knew of any nude modeling jobs.) The CBS
Morning News called to ask if I'd like to be on. I said yes enthusiastically
and asked whether I should wear my tux or my loincloth. They called back and
cancelled me and used John Sununu instead.
Still
desperate for publicity, I hired a small plane to tow a physics equation on
which I'd been working over Denver and Boulder. The CBS Morning News had been
my best chance to become famous, and I'd blown it. (Never waking up in time to
see the morning news, I hadn't understood what CBS wanted.) I decided to take
advantage of my continuing anonymity and return to high school one last time.
7. HIGH SCHOOL
Borrowing from my problem student file, I forged about 40 documents and went
from being a 26-year-old undergrad to being 17-year-old Gilligan Rosner (so
named after the sitcom character by a divorced and bitter--and
imaginary--mother). Typically, the school system to which I submitted the
documents lost them. Using four leftover documents, I began summer school. (I
needed to repeat a couple classes I'd missed during my imaginary junior year
after a fictitious auto accident sent me into a fabricated coma.)
By day, I attended class; by night, I made a living delivering stripping
telegrams and bouncing. I got good grades in most of my courses--due to my
advanced age relative to my classmates, I had a functional IQ of about 250. I
didn't try to seduce any students, though many people my age, including some
teachers, had few qualms about hitting on people in their mid-teens. I did
take a nice girl to the homecoming dance, stopping on the way to deliver a
stripping telegram while she sat in the car.
Each day I expected to be caught. The stress accelerated my hair loss, and one
of my classmates elected me "most bald." Gilligan scored a 1580 on
the SAT and treated his calculus class to cookies. At the end of the fall
semester, I transferred to New York to be with my girlfriend, who became
Gilligan's legal guardian. Because I was taking calculus, I was placed in a
science magnet school in Spanish Harlem, where I aroused widespread suspicion.
Some students called out "Five-oh!" (as in Hawaii 5-0) to indicate
that I was a narc, but I had some immunity since few narcs score 1580. Had I
not been taking calculus and not had high SAT's, I might have been sent to my
neighborhood school, the bloodcurdling West Side High, where someone surely
would have shot me behind the ear. Math skills saved my life.
After graduation, I was looking for work posing naked when I was accidentally
hired by the MTV game show Remote Control, where I wrote physics questions
starring The Brady Bunch and Elvis. While the show was taping in Florida, several cast and crewmembers got tattoos. Mine reads "BORN TO DO MATH."
I used my increased television savvy to get on Geraldo and, with Kevin Langdon,
on Morton Downey, Jr.
My fiance and I moved to L.A. Carole got an excellent job, and, when deals
with several TV producers fell through, I returned to bouncing, nude modeling,
and SAT tutoring. The world's best plastic surgery is done in L.A., and I had some hair transplants to reinforce a hairline that was slowly recovering
from high school.
Omni printed the Titan Test. I figured that Ron and Scot Morris wouldn't be
eager to give a high-IQ sleazeball any further publicity, so I borrowed my
fiance's last name, and the mild-mannered Richard Sterman was born. He got a
perfect score and fittingly fell into a publicity vacuum. The Guinness Book no
longer runs IQ data, and Omni failed to respond to a small campaign to get them
to publish mini-profiles of the top scorers. Inspired by his Titan score and
by a comment made by Sununu years ago on CBS, Sterman attempted the Mega Test,
not surprisingly beating my score by several points.
Unmolested by media attention, I've continued to pose for art classes and have
accumulated 1,180 +/- 15 acts of public nudity. At least 15,000 people have
seen me naked, not counting Geraldo viewers. I work the doors of two bars and
am probably the best bouncer in American history at being able to catch
underage patrons, having snagged 3,600 +/- 150 bogus ID's, nailed another 5,000
underage people sneaking in without any ID, and developed a complete
statistical model of fake ID's. Having been lightly pummeled by a frenzied bar
customer earlier this month, I'm aware that my other bouncer skills could be
improved.
Recently, I looked my fiance in the eye and asked if living with the possessor
of one of America's highest IQ's intimidated her. She laughed and laughed.
LETTER FROM RON HOEFLIN
P.O. Box 539
New York, NY 10101
Jan. 4, 1991
Dear Richard Sterman:
Enclosed is a mailing list for Noesis. Sixteen people are full members,
twenty people are non-member subscribers. You have the option of excluding
these 20 subscribers from participation if you prefer.
Several people paid for issues 57-62. Rather than refunding their $10, I am
offering them 12 issues of my journal In-Genius, unless they specifically
request a $10 refund.
You should request dues from all of the members and subscribers when you
publish your January issue. I've been charging $10 per 6 issues, but you have
the option of altering this amount. You should emphasize that even those who
have already paid for issues 57-62 should send you $10, since I am no longer
editor. They should write to me if they want a $10 refund rather than 12
issues of In-Genius.
George Dicks has not paid dues for the last 6 issues of Noesis, so I am
only going to send him the last 4 of these issues if and when he sends me $10.
How you handle his dues payments is up to you. Enclosed is some material I
just received from him for publication in Noesis.
Sincerely,
Ron
Hoeflin
P.S. I have not included myself on the enclosed list of members and
subscribers. I'm willing to continue for the next six months if your dues
request is not excessive.
******
Comments from the editor (Rosner):
All 37 subscribers (and any other interested parties) are encouraged to
participate. Ten dollars for six issues still seems reasonable. Obviously,
everybody will receive the January issue. To receive further issues send
checks for $10 to Rick Rosner, 5139 Balboa Blvd. #303, Encino, CA 91316-3430.
NOESIS
49 REBUTTAL
George W. Dicks, Jr.
198 Sturm St.
New Haven, IN 46774
(219) 749-8511
I would like to take a moment to clarify certain issues regarding my paper
which was published in Noesis 49. Please don't take my remarks as being
any more than they are intended to be, a clarification of some of the points I
was attempting to make.
In this rebuttal I will show the following:
1) That contrary to the review published in Noesis 49 of my solution, defining a solution as that work which was presented for the approval of the society and not as potential work derivable from a given method, is indeed more general than the one offered in Noesis 44.
2) That, contrary to the review published in Noesis 49, my solution actually contains references to all elements of the problem, taking each of these into account in producing a general solution for this type of problem.
3) That while I do not dispute CTMU, this does not mean that I find it to be completely satisfactory which should be obvious in that I chose to write a paper of my own.
4) That it is indeed reasonable to distinguish between the predictor and the chooser, treating the predictor's ability to predict as a skill which can be measured empirically just as any other skill may be measured. Further, this is the only way to eliminate a computational regression.
5)
That, while the review focuses on the method which I gave to estimate the
predictor's predictive ability, this method is actually a relatively small part
of the paper and was almost omitted. Also, that the reason I almost omitted
the method was due to possible mathematical difficulties and not to
philosophical considerations which in this case are almost prima facie.
Some Comments
First,
I must admit that Chris Langan's paper on the subject was the first time that I
had ever heard of this problem. Also, I must admit that my knowledge of some
of the finer points he was attempting to make is nowhere near the level of
his. However, allowing these caveats, I feel my paper is as good a solution as
I've seen.
Second, I believe we all owe Chris a round of applause for his detailed
treatment of this particular problem, not to mention his dedicated editorship
these few months. I'm sure we all agree that the society is better off because
he is a member.
Third, I would like to thank Chris Cole for introducing this problem to the
group. I have been reading the past issues of the journal and I can see that
this is not the only excellent problem which he has placed before the society.
Thank you Chris.
Finally, I would like to thank the membership as a whole. The past few months
I have made several new friends and have spoken with several of you on the
telephone. I would have contacted all of you except about half the members
have unlisted phones and/or live in post boxes. Because you have gone to such
trouble to maintain your privacy, I have and will continue to respect your
wishes. As always you have my phone and address and I would like to hear from
any of you.
Rebuttal--Point 1
(Noesis 49 solution is more complete than Noesis 44)
Please
understand that I am focusing completely upon the solutions as they were
actually presented. The premise of this point of contention was, and remains,
that the Noesis 44 solution omitted many of the potential solutions to
the problem.
Please recall that the Noesis 44 solution focuses on the case where the
Predictor either controls the behavior of the Chooser, by inserting some
modification into its programming, or merely examines this programming via
something analogous to a logic probe.
While these are both very correct solutions to the problem of how Predictor can
have such an unusually successful record of prediction, they both assume that
Predictor is the active party, actively examining Chooser's programming and
altering it if such alteration would be beneficial to Predictor's cause.
There is, however, an alternative solution, namely that where Chooser alters
Predictor's programming. Another alternative solution is the case where
Predictor gains some knowledge of the choice via some method other than direct
observation of the programming involved. This is similar to the behaviorist
interpretation of psychology where only observable behaviors are considered.
Other alternative cases include the possibility that neither player is the
active influence. Rather, some external programmer may in fact be the real
active force and the two players are merely inactive puppets for his amusement.
Another alternative, the most probable one, is that some combination of these
cases is at work. In other words, the Predictor, Chooser, and every other
player in the universe is attempting to ascertain and control each other's
programming.
Admittedly, all of these cases have representations within Chris Langan's
CTMU. The important thing which must be realized is that to resolve to a
solution one or more of the players must become dominant over the other
players. Until this occurs a chaotic exchange of move and counter-move will
occur in the computational universe.
This is why I maintain that the Noesis 44 solution is incomplete as
presented. By focusing only on the cases where the Predictor is the active
party and the Chooser is just along for the ride, the Noesis 44 solution
ignores a vast array of interesting solutions.
I maintain that the Noesis 49 solution can successfully resolve all of
these because the probability of success can be determined empirically based
upon the data generated by the previous experimental runs. Contrary to the
review published in Noesis 49, the Noesis 49 solution demands
statistics on the conditions surrounding each experimental run as they enable
finer and finer estimates of the probabilities involved to be calculated. Empiricism
works. Anything less is just theology.
Rebuttal--Point 2
(Noesis 49 solution is complete)
In
the review of my paper the editor makes several minor errors stating that my
solution is in some small ways incomplete. I will deal with each of these in
turn.
1) The editor states that I have omitted one of the possible rewards offered in
Newcomb's Paradox. (Pg 9, Pa 3) This is clearly false as can be observed by
examining clause 4 of my solution to the paradox on page 8. Here I state if
PredictedChoice = {Box2}
OfferedRewards = {1000, 1,000,000} otherwise
OfferedRewards = {1000, 0}
I
am sorry if my notation caused anyone any difficulty. PotentialRewards includes
the set of all possible values which may be used in setting the
OfferedRewards. I should probably have enclosed 0 and 1,000,000 in another set
of brackets, indicating that they are a mutually exclusive set of choices.
OfferedRewards is set based upon a rule, unique for each type of game. In
Newcomb's Paradox the rule is that mentioned immediately above. OfferedRewards
is the set of all rewards to which Chooser has access. It is a subset of all
possible rewards, PotentialRewards. ActualReward is a subset of OfferedRewards
and is the set rewards actually taken. If PredictedChoice = {Box2}
ActualReward is a subset of {1,000 1,000,000} which could = 1,001,000 otherwise
ActualReward is a subset of {1000, 0}
2) The editor states that one of the cases which I list under the heading of
Minimalist Choices is contradictory to the formalism (Pg 9, Pa 4). The case in
question is the last paragraph of page 3. The contradiction is to clause 3 of
the formalism presented in paragraph 2. It is true that this does represent a
contradiction but not a terribly devastating one. The real problem lies in
clause 3 which is actually too specific. This clause actually only applies to
a rather small set of problems such as Newcomb's Paradox where R1 is bracketed
by R2,1 and R2,2. If I had been more careful in proofreading I would have
caught this and removed the statements about the relative values of the rewards
in clause 3. The minimalist Choices, which maybe should be called Common Sense
Choices, are presented as I intended, however.
This brings up the question of why they were even included in the first place.
I was almost ashamed to include such obvious conclusions as the fact that
Chooser should always avoid negative rewards, given the audience of the paper,
but in the interest of completeness I went ahead and included them. Please do
not consider your intelligence insulted in any way.
******
Editor's comment: This has been the first half of George Dicks's NOESIS 49
REBUTTAL. I'll run the remainder of the article in the next issue. The
following is a letter from George Dicks that accompanied his article.
To whom it may concern,
Ron Hoeflin called me the other day asking if I would be interested in sharing
the Noesis editorship with Chris Langan and himself. I agreed and am
now announcing such. Having gotten that out of the way I also would like to
describe some ideas which I have on how the journal should be handled.
First, such a journal is the community property of all members of our society.
In other words, given our large geographical dispersal this journal is the only
tie binding the society into a cohesive whole. As such, I believe it stands to
reason that the society can be no more successful than its journal whose
success is determined based largely upon the participation of its readership.
Second, the journal should have regular features to consistently catch the
attention of the readership. This is the way a newspaper is structured. For
instance, when my wife brings in the Sunday Journal Gazette I always begin with
the editorials followed by Marilyn's column in Parade followed by Outland,
Doonesbury, and Far Side. I also read whatever catches my eye, typically
scientific articles, while I'm finding these features. In other words, they
provide incentive for scanning material which otherwise I might totally ignore.
Finally, we need to expand both our scope of influence and our visibility. As
many of our members have implied, doing well on some test is a pretty pathetic
excuse for an organization to exist. On the contrary, we should provide positive
role models of thinking men and women. Furthermore, we would probably interest
more prospective members if we came out of the intellectual closet and quit
worrying about our teapots and whatever storms they contain. I will reiterate
that this group has mental firepower far in excess of what our meager numbers
would suggest. It therefore makes sense that we contribute to the world at
large.
Now having given these concerns, let me lay out a plan for meeting them.
I suggest that the society return to the practice of rotating responsibility
for producing the essays contained within the various monthly issues. While it
seems that strong, central editorship is necessary to provide a vehicle for
reader feedback via an editorial page or something similar; it is also clear
that central editorship can strangle reader input if not handled in the proper
fashion. I propose that each issue contain two or three major essays of 4-8
pages prepared by the members on a rotating basis. this will encourage all of
the members to contribute without making undue pressures on the time of any
particular member.
Secondly, I propose that each member prepare a one page, short essay for each
issue. In this way we can get to know each other better and we just might get
a more varied and interesting journal. I envision these one-pagers evolving
into columns similar to Marilyn's column in Parade or Scot Morris's column in
Omni. We could thus sample the various interests of the membership and just
might learn a thing or two. I also see the readers giving these authors
feedback about previously discussed topics. All in all, this could be quite
interesting.
In order to console the gods of logistics, I propose that the members print
their various essays and forward sufficient copies to the editor that each
member will receive one. The editor would then bundle these essays, along with
the letters to the editor and a table of contents, into booklet form and mail a
copy of the journal to each member. While some may fear that this could lead to
a riotous assemblage of type styles and print quality I feel that the benefit
of increasing the scope of the journal to include input from all members while
not taxing the time or financial resources of any individual member far outweighs
any such concern. The annual subscription rate would then be a mere ten
dollars which would cover postage, letters to the editor, a cover, and a
protective envelope for each issue.
Finally, I see us publishing a yearly anthology of the best essays submitted
during a given year. I see such an anthology selling for 5-7 dollars and being
distributed through outlets such as B. Dalton. While this may seem like a
rather outlandish idea, I believe it offers many advantages. First, if the
project is successful we can easily recoup any and all costs involved in
production of the journal. If it is reasonably successful we could even
upgrade our yearly meeting and possibly even help our more distant members with
travel expenses. Second, the project would allow us to gain name recognition
and possibly attract new members. These things alone indicate that the idea
might be worth pursuing.
Here is how I believe such a work should be structured: First, we begin with an
introduction to the society followed by one-page biographies of the members.
After the bios would come the 12 best long and 36 best short essays published
during the previous year. These would be selected by some form of plebescite of
the membership. After the essays would be an obsolete entrance exam (with
answers) and the current entrance exam (without). In this way interested
parties could attempt to join the society.
Well, that's about it. I would appreciate it if each of you would respond with
your critiques on these ideas as well as further suggestions of your own.
Yours
Truly,
George
W. Dicks, Jr.
NEWCOMB'S
PARADOX AND OTHER QUESTIONS
with addenda by your editor
For people who walked in late, here is Newcomb's Paradox as it was presented by
Chris Cole in this journal's second issue, back in April, 1986. Members have
been yammering about it ever since, with increasingly stultifying resluts
(typo, but I like it).
A being put one thousand dollars in box A and either zero or one million
dollars in box B and presents you with two choices:
(1) Open box B only.
(2) Open both box A and box B.
The being put money in box B only if it predicted you will choose option (1).
The being put nothing in box B if it predicted you will do anything other than
choose option (1), including choosing option (2), flipping a coin, etc.
Assuming you have never known the being to be wrong in predicting your actions,
which option should you choose to maximize the amount of money you get?
Publishing this paradox was like the time on Star Trek (I hate Star Trek) when
the Enterprise's computer went crazy and Spock shut it down by instructing it
to compute the last digit of pi. Or how Einstein wasted big chunks of his life
fighting quantum physics. Discussion of this and the marbles problem has
constipated Noesis intolerably. I know how I'd choose, but I'm not
telling.
I don't want to hear much more about this problem. You guys have had almost
five years to get it right. Send me anything but the most clear, concise and
self-contained analysis, and I'll edit it like crazy or tell you to wait until
someone else is editor. My brain takes enough abuse when people punch me in
the face.
For readers who prefer simpler problems, here are a couple of questions I wrote
for a TV game show which were actually posed to not-overly-bright
contestants. Good luck.
Q. WHAT FORCE OF NATURE KEEPS CANINE STAR BENJI FROM HURTLING INTO SPACE?
A. GRAVITY
Q. NAME A TYPE OF ANIMAL OTHER THAN WEASELS.
A. ANYTHING BUT WEASELS, I.E. SHEEP, TROUT, POSSUM
Here are a couple questions which were designed to stump our contestants.
Answers next month.
Q. WHAT IS THE TECHNICAL TERM FOR THE DESIRE TO EAT CLAY?
Hint: It's a 13-letter word.
Q. THE MEMBERS OF THE BRADY BUNCH (an insipid sitcom family) ARE EACH IN THEIR
OWN PERSONAL SPACESHIP, SPEEDING AWAY FROM EARTH IN A STRAIGHT LINE. RELATIVE
TO EARTH, MR. BRADY IS TRAVELING AT HALF THE SPEED OF LIGHT. RELATIVE TO MR.
BRADY, LOVELY CAROL BRADY (Florence Henderson) IS TRAVELING AT HALF THE SPEED
OF LIGHT. ALICE, THE HOUSEKEEPER, IS MOVING AT HALF LIGHTSPEED RELATIVE TO
MRS. BRADY, AS IS SAM THE BUTCHER, ALICE'S BOYFRIEND, RELATIVE TO ALICE. GREG IS MOVING AT HALF LIGHTSPEED RELATIVE TO SAM, AS ARE MARSHA RELATIVE TO GREG,
PETER RELATIVE TO MARSHA, JAN (Eve Plumb) RELATIVE TO PETER, BOBBY RELATIVE TO
JAN, LITTLE CINDY BRADY RELATIVE TO BOBBY, AND TIGER, THE FAMILY DOG (who
co-starred with Don Johnson in the film version of the Harlan Ellison story
"A Boy and His Dog"), RELATIVE TO CINDY. HOW FAST IS TIGER MOVING
RELATIVE TO THE EARTH?
Hint: Relative to Earth, Mrs. Brady is moving at 4/5 lightspeed.
THOUGHTS ON CTMU
Chris Cole
C. M. Langan has proposed a Computation-Theoretic Model of the Universe (CTMU,
for short). Basically, the idea is that the universe is a computer, or at
least that natural processes are computations. This idea is not new. Ed
Fredkin has been proposing it for years, and readers of science fiction will
recognize it from the works of Douglas Adams, among others. Feynman was asked
by Smithsonian Magazine of his assessment of Fredkin: he replied that while Ed
was a personal friend, he was an "incautious thinker."
How are we to evaluate the idea that the universe is a computer? Is this a
theory or a definition? If it is a theory, what evidence is offered? I have
not seen any. If it is a definition, then how can it solve anything?
In particular, Chris Langan uses CTMU to solve the marble problem and the
Newcomb problem, both in ways that I do not agree with. Chris' arguments are
stated in very general terms, so I cannot follow them. Perhaps Chris could
solve a simpler problem using only very specific terms. So far I am forced to
repeat Quine's objection: "that is just ambiguous enough to sound plausible
and to prevent further discussion."
RANDOM
WALKS AT WORK
Rick Rosner/Sterman
(This article is an example of the sloppily-reasoned stuff I wouldn't mind
seeing from y'all. Don't some of you have silly little theories like this?)
Einstein, I think, proved the square root law for random walks, that the
average distance of a randomly-moving object from its point of origin is
proportional to the square root of the time it's been moving. (Whether he
proved it or not, he used it in his paper on Brownian motion.)
I think that there is a similar, though more ridiculous, law for experience in
a novel environment. I've worked in more than 100 different places at a number
of different sleazy, sometimes risky jobs, so, basically, I'm always starting a
new job and often asking myself, "Do I really have a clue?"
I convince myself that I do have a clue both by choosing jobs that a
well-trained monkey could handle and by using my square root rules of thumb for
work experience. I believe that expertise on a particular job is roughly
proportional to the square root of the amount of time spent on the job--that
after nine weeks on a job, a person will have three times the clues after one
week. I believe that the number of novel situations encountered while on a job
is proportional to the reciprocal of the square root of the cumulative time
spent on the job.
These rules, of course, are superficial and don't take into account varying
degrees of competence among workers, the changing nature of jobs over time, and
a zillion other things, but they're still fun to use. Is it unreasonable to
think that someone with 20 years of experience in a particular field has about
twice the expertise of someone with 5 years' experience and that the 20-year
person will find him/herself winging it only about half as often as the 5-year
person?
MEET
THE SIMPSONS
Not Bart, Maggie, and Homer, but Simpson's Index, a formula for measuring the
diversity of fauna in a particular ecosphere. (I suppose it's also used in
lots of other places.) An acre of land that has 1,711 mice and two woodchucks
has less diversity than an acre that has 148 mice, 6 beavers, 97 sparrows, a
moose, 8 raccoons, 22 shrews, 19 prairie dogs, 4 rabbits, and 3 coyotes.
Simpson's Index lets you assign a number to that diversity, and it's a very
cool formula.
All you do is take a census, square the percentages into which your population
is divided, add 'em up, and take the reciprocals. Simpson's Index for the
mouse/woodchuck population is slightly more than one. For the more diverse
population, it's the reciprocal of the quantity 148/308 squared plus 6/308
squared plus 97/308 squared etc. It equals about 2.95.
Simpson's Index is fun to misapply. You could, for example, use it to evaluate
the diversity of your sexual experience. Say an acquaintance of mine has had
1000 sexual incidents, 300 with person A, 200 each with persons B and C, 50
each with D, E, and F, 15 each with 5 other people, 5 each with 10 people, and
once with each of 25 other people. (This acquaintance was late to enter the
safe sex era.) The diversity of my acquaintance's experience equals the
reciprocal of the sum of 90000 plus 2 x 40000 plus 3 x 2500 plus 5 x 225 plus
10 x 25 plus 25 each divided by one million. This equals 1000000/178900 or
about 5.6.
I use Simpson's Index to evaluate the diversity of my work experience within a
particular (low-rent) field. I've spent about 1,600 nights working in bars,
900 nights in bar A, 300 in B, 100 each in C and D, 50 each in two other bars,
20 each in three other bars, the rest scattered and irrelevant in terms of the
calculation. Simpson's Index equals 2560000/(810000 + 90000 + 20000 + 5000 +
1200), about 2 3/4.
COMBINING
SQUARE ROOT RULES OF THUMB AND SIMPSON'S INDEX
I think you can multiply the amount of time spent in a particular field by the
square root of Simpson's Index to get a rough equivalent to your expertise had
you spent all your time in one place. For example, my fiance has spent about 20
months in each of three jobs as a purchasing agent. Her total time in
purchasing is five years. Simpson's Index of the diversity of her experience is
about three. So I figure she knows as much after five years in three places as
if she spent five times the square root of three equals eight and a half years
in just one place.
Is it unreasonable to think that someone who spent three years doing similar
jobs in each of four places has about the same abstract expertise as someone
who's worked in the same place for 24 years? That is, if each of these people
was booted from his/her present job, one after a total of 12 years in 4 places
and the other after 24 years in just one place, wouldn't you expect them to
have roughly equivalent expertise when starting new, similar jobs? If not,
what would be your formula, or is this just wasting your time?
Anyway, my rules of thumb for work expertise boil down to taking the square
root of time spent in a job field and multiplying it by the fourth root of
employment diversity as calculated by Simpson's Index. Returning to my bar
experience, 1600 nights in bars with a diversity of 2 3/4 equals an expertise
level of the square root of 1600 times the fourth root of 2.75. This equals 40
times 1.29, about 52. I should have twice the expertise of someone with 675
nights' experience in one place and an experise level of about 26, three
times the expertise of someone with 300 nights' experience, four times the
expertise of someone who's worked 170 nights. I should be dealing with novel
situations about 1/52 of the time. (Last time I worked, a guy unzipped
himself in the middle of the dance floor and peed all over the other dancers.
That was novel.) Someone with half the expertise should see new stuff 4% of
the time; with a third the expertise, 6% of the time; with one-fourth the
expertise, 8% of the time. (This means most jobs quickly become very
uninteresting.)
Reasonable? Ridiculous? Let me know.
*Chris Cole says that Ron wasn't bugged by my obnoxiousness.